1 1 >> >>: hello, everybody. Welcome. Today is day two of 5 the conference, better learning by design. Thank you so much for waiting patiently as we've just been getting everything together. I'm just going to 10 open the conference today with a few details as to logistics. And then we'll get us on to our keynote speaker, David Rose. So I'd just like to say welcome to 15 the new participants today and also the veterans who were here yesterday. You can get all the details from the veterans as to what's good around Davis Center. 20 And isn't also like to remind you that at our registration table, you'll find our members of our UDL team in case you have any questions and the 25 registration table is where you 2 1 got your badges this morning. And also bathrooms are right near the registration table. In any case if you don't find 5 anyone there at the registration table, you can find members of our team at the Boulder room just down the hall. So as for an overview of the day today, 10 we're going to start with our keynote speaker, David Rose, and then we're going to have a brief break and continue with our keynote speaker until about 15 12:00. Afterwards we're going to have a lunch break right out here lunch will be served. If you do want food that isn't available here, there is food 20 for purchase downstairs at the marketplace and that is open until 2 p.m. Afterwards, we will have a breakout session from 1 to 2:15, and in our 25 breakout sessions, the 3 1 presenters can feel free to move the chairs and tables around, so that it's appropriate for discussion or for any type of 5 configuration that is best for that particular session. And also, we will be video recording, just for educational purposes and there are consent 10 forms on the table, so if the presenter of each breakout session would just fill out that consent form and specify their level of comfort with that video 15 recording, that would be great. Also, if members in the breakout sessions can fill out evaluations, which are also on the table, as paper form, or 20 they can do them online, so either one of those would be great and Holly Parker will be showing us in a few minutes as to how to fill out an evaluation 25 online. After our first 4 1 breakout session from 1 to 2:15 we'll have another break for about 15 minutes and then we'll have a second breakout session 5 from 2:30 to 3:45 and actually today is kind of special, because at the end of the day today from 4:30 to 6, we will have a formal social hour around 10 networking hour, which will be in the Livak ballroom. There will be a cash bar. There will be posters and vendors and exhibiter tables there and also 15 there will be prized so this is actually quite important. Veterans will tell you yesterday we had a great prize session yesterday and we're continuing 20 it today. So the rule is the three P's. First you must post on the blog which Holly's going to get to in a minute. The second rule is you must be 25 present at the social hour, the 5 1 networking hour, and the third rule is if you are present, and you did post, you have the opportunity to get a prize. And 5 we have a prize available today that is valued over $600, so if I were you I would definitely get there. 10 >> >>: let's see. I guess right now I would like to bring up Holly Parker to talk about the blog and about evaluations. 15 >> >>: we would like some clue as about the prize. >> >>: Oakes OHH, OK I'm going to think about that one. 20 >> >>: could I get a little help for a second? >> >>: I see David has his 25 keynote here all ready to go, so 6 1 I don't want to mess with it. Good morning everybody, I've already had a few questions about how to get to the blog. 5 There is a direct URL to the blog, but I think the simplest way would be just to go through our website for UDL@UVM he so I'm going to he show that route. 10 The put in the regular UVM .EDU and then the TILDE sign, and then universal design. So then there is the conference ling. So if you click on that, that 15 will take you to the blog. That's kind of the easiest route. Now, I am quite pleased at the amount of postings that have come up in the water 20 cooler. Even this morning, I get all the emails as the ADMIN. So I saw all of the conversation that had started, and what I'm going to show you just briefly 25 is the water cooler link, which 7 1 you get to over here on the right-hand side and then you can see there are 20 responses so far. This area is set up as a 5 threaded discussion. So if you see something that you would like to reply to, all you have to do is click on the reply link that is associated with that 10 particular post. For example, Hailey posted something about getting the slides from yesterday. And you can see that I replied to her this morning 15 and it shows the thread by indentation here. So that's just a way to kind of navigate through the discussion here, and be able to reply to a particular 20 post if you're interested. Some people have posted questions, so feel free to jump in, click that reply button and answer them if you're able to. The other thing 25 I wanted to show you is the way 8 1 that you go to an evaluation. So we're on day two this morning, and if we click that link over here on the right, 5 then you can see the schedule of the day, and underneath each of the sessions, there should be an evaluation link associated with that particular session. For 10 example, here's the link for David Rose's keynote evaluation, and if you click there, you will be taken to a brief survey monkey survey. So we're asking 15 that if you're able to do these online, that's the most efficient way for us to get the feedback very quickly. But we also accept the paper 20 evaluations that are located in each of the rooms, and you can pick one up either before or after each of the sessions. There is a box for the 25 evaluations at the welcome table 9 1 that Puja was mentioning, so you can just drop your evaluation in that box. 5 >> >>: and I think there's still a computer located out by the welcome table, a laptop computer, that you can use, as well, to fill out the 10 evaluations or check your email if you need to check email. >> >>: Puja's feeding me some pointers here. There's also 15 some computers on the lower levels of the Davis Center, Level 3 and 1, I believe? So if I can help you in any way to get on the blog throughout the day 20 today, just let me know. And also, if if you have any questions or you notice that something is missing that you'd like up on the blog, presenters, 25 if you have not yet submitted 10 1 your slides or if you have submitted them and they are not appearing on today's sessions, appear on the blog, please send 5 an email to me with the attachment and it's Holly . Parker at UVM .EDU. >> >>: OK. Thank you, Holly. 10 One more thing I wanted to mention is that this, the vendors and the posters and the exhibitors will actually be available and presenting in in 15 the frank Livak room all day today so if you have a few minutes during a break or if you just want to take a break from a session, you can go in there and 20 check out what's available. >> >>: lastly, we have rides that are available to the Sheraton at 6:15, and they will 25 be leaving here. If you would 11 1 like a ride, please do go to the registration table and sign and let one of our team members know about that. I would like to 5 introduce Charlie, a member of our team who has been working in education for over 46 years, and working at UVM for over 40 years. Thank you. 10 >> >>: some of the more mature people in the audience will remember Jonathan winters who had a great routine about, I've 15 been in this job for over a quarter of a century. That was one of his characters. I haven't gotten to the half-century mark yet. But 20 thank you, Puja, for that very brief introduction. I appreciate that. >> >>: I was part of the UDL 25 team that went to Wakefield 12 1 Mass. last spring to work for three days with cast. And my last memory of seeing David was at the end of the training, and 5 David followed us out, actually everybody kind of followed us out, to the doorway between the cast offices and the rest of the world. And we stood there and 10 in our good goodbyes, we kind of dreamed up this conference. The doorway space is called a lumen, and in some ways it's a place between two realities, and for 15 us it was the reality of cast and the reality of what would come after that. We had spent three days immersed in that bridge between the neuroscience 20 of learning and the possibilities maybe of working on higher education with largely a population of professors who had no construction whatsoever 25 in pedagogy. So it was a huge 13 1 challenge. But that's what lumens are, they're places of possibility. They're places between parentheses of 5 experience around I think that's the way the team took it and I think it's the way David meant it when he and skip and grace and the rest of the cast staff 10 sent us off. >> >>: I see David as an applied theorist, very much a neuroscientist and very much a 15 person embedded in the lives of children, youth, adolescents, college-age students, and their teachers, whoever their teachers may be. I think it's a tough 20 job to be an applied theorist. It's much more comfortable to be either applied tore a theorist, but to live in both of those worlds opens yourself up for 25 expert criticism from the other 14 1 world. And you're always kind of pushing the boundaries. David's very much a boundary-pusher and he's very 5 much a bridger. And I'd like to just spend a couple of minutes reading you some of the titles of his most recent publications, because they'll show you what 10 that bridging is all about. >> >>: applying universal design for learning with children in poverty. So there's universal 15 design, there's learning, and then there are these kids in our schools that forever we've been trying to figure out how to close not only achievement gap, 20 but the gap of affect for them, so that they can experience school with the same joy and relevance as other children. 25 >> >>: so there is one bridge. 15 1 >> >>: there's echoes of Michael Harrington in that title, for those of you who remember Michael Harrington. 5 >> >>: another title: Is synthesis possible? Making doubly sure in research and application. There's that 10 bridge again between research and application, and the view of the scientist. Making doubly sure. 15 >> >>: here's another favorite of mine. Cognition of learning, meeting the challenge of individual differences. You know, we use that term 20 individual differences as if there's nobody else in the world but you can't have an individual difference without a group or a larger other. And so the 25 consideration of individual 16 1 differences is also partly that bridge between who is this individual we're talking about and what are we comparing them 5 to? So this world of bridging is something I appreciate in immense ways that David brings with him. As I hear David talk, I hear historical bridges. I 10 hear historical bridges to PIAGET, one of the great thinkers of how we think, how we learn, conditions for learning, conditions for extremely 15 positive mental development. Way before there was any way of accessing what was lighting up when the brain was used in certain ways. There's bridges 20 to the GODSKEY, there's bridges to the educational theorists who have been around for a long time and I'm stalking talking like a thousand years who have always 25 enacting what is being learned 17 1 in the moment it to deepening your understanding of any particular reality that you're living in. So I am so grateful 5 that David and skip, but this morning David, have continued their commitment to this project in UDL at the University of Vermont, and have continued 10 their willingness to come support us, support you in the work that you're doing here in the Davis Center this morning. Skip? David? Where are you? 15 There you are. >> >>: thank you, Charlie, for that very sweet introduction. I guess I'm on a lumen, too, that 20 was great. Very nice. I apologize. I won't be able to stay for the nice events at the end of the day. I'm in a week of sort of great stress and 25 great opportunity, just finished 18 1 turning in my graze yesterday, so you university faculty know what that feels like. And you always have that student that 5 you just don't know, is it going to come in by 5 or not, and then turned up up in my grades and rushed up here and I have to leave to go down to Washington 10 tomorrow doing a congressional briefing on matters that are of interest to all of us here, I'll tell you all about that toward the end of the day. But there's 15 sort of action everywhere, but my assistant was saying, are you really going to drive up four hours, you know, come back four hours and make it in time to get 20 on your plane and get to Washington and all that? And I thought this would actually feel quite nice, you know, kind of settle down for a while in my 25 car and think about this. I'm 19 1 hoping, skip said you were a nice audience, you're not going to attack me or anything, so I am actually very much looking 5 forward to spending the time with you, and I have plenty of time which often doesn't happen. Because there's a little breakout session later, so I'll 10 be putting things often in a, you know, in a garage where I'll say, well, maybe let's talk about that in the afternoon session, but it's enough time to 15 do some broad strokes now, and still have some time for a discussion later for a few of you that like to do that. Just broad what I'm going to do 20 today. This morning I'm going to do background on UDL, some of the underlying learning sciences. And a few application, although not too 25 much of that. And I'll take a 20 1 break and I'll also see from the wonderful colleagues at UVM just whether I should tweak what I'm doing for the second part. The 5 second part is largely I'm going to talk about my own teaching, what I do and don't do and I want to say before I begin that I actually had a disappointing 10 semester. I tried some things that didn't work, that I'll tell it you about, a couple of things that did work that I'll tell you about, and I also had a 15 neurological incident myself, which made teaching very hard for a while, and I'll tell you a little bit about that. So it was a kind of like odd semester. 20 And I thought I would be a lot better than I was, but I thought this is a good group to kind of share that with, that actually for a little while I was a 25 different person. Things that 21 1 were easy before were not easy for me all of a sudden, and I, too, like you, had some bad teaching days where I was not on 5 top of it, didn't do very well and some things that I'd hoped to do were not great. So we're all too human, and so I'll tell about the sort of good things 10 and bad things that happened this semester. And show you some student work and things like that. So broad theory and some application for the first 15 part, little break, my own teaching which are graduate students in education, and then more interchange in the afternoon, OK? And I like being 20 interrupted with questions. I will often ignore them, so don't worry. That is, if I don't think it's sort of in the thrust of where we need to be in the 25 moment, I'll say let's talk 22 1 about that a little bit later, so you can feel free to ask them, knowing that I'm going to feel free I don't want to answer 5 that right now, OK? But hopefully el I'll be able to later. And let's see if I get up on this screen. This me shifting or do you shift? 10 >> >>: oh, you are I'm already ahead. Let me get out my little magic wand which I've never used. Oh, I see. I'm looking 15 over there. But -- now I get it. I'm already having technical difficulties. This is where I want you to look. And is it -- it's a little, we're 20 going do some brain slides so I hope that it's bright enough. Is it possible if I needed to, to dim the lights? Let's just see, let me try T I just want to 25 see it before I get going. 23 1 Yeah, OK, now, is that enough for interpreting? Do you have enough light? 5 >> >>: that's good? OK. All right. >> >>: so, I want to do some broad setting. There we go. I 10 think skip probably talked about NIMAS, I'm not going to do much talking about NIMAS. Did you talk about NIMAS, skip? 15 >> >>: a little bit >> >>: and did you talk about the higher Ed extension or not? 20 >> >>: not yet snoovment is it all right if I do that? Or are you going to do it tomorrow? >> >>: no, no, OK. 25 24 1 >> >>: NIMAS, skip would have introduced you to, and so I can skip that for the moment, except I want to go into it a little 5 bit. One part of it. So NIMAS is a standard for digital source files that can be used to accurately and reliably produce instructional materials in a 10 variety of alternate formats using the same source file, the glory of XML, you make a thing once, display it in many different ways. It's the kind 15 of thing we couldn't do in the world of print but now we can. Partly I want to focus on this. What does it do? 20 >> >>: I actually think, more important than the technical standard, which NIMAS really is a technical standard, make your books like this, so we can make 25 lots of different things out of 25 1 them, was the putting into congressional language the word print disability. And I want to talk about that word, actually. 5 For most it will it will underlie most of what I'm going to talk about. Print disability is way too narrow for what we're going to ultimately talk about. 10 But I think it's a watershed moment, the inclusion of this word, and I want to talk about why that is. OK? 15 >> >>: the outcomes of NIMAS is that virtually every textbook in American schools, this is K-12 published after 2006 is now available in a digital XML 20 version to any child with a print disability. So there's a law, there's a force, there's a definition that people care about, if you have a print 25 disability, then you have access 26 1 to materials that other kids do not, and that you didn't used to. So every textbook since 2006 is now available as a 5 digital source file marked up in the way that actually skip's large commission defined. And if I can say it, skip can tell you more about it tomorrow 10 because skip's in charge of this, but we've been asked just, is it OK to say this? >> >>: sure, why not. 15 >> >>: I think it's OK to say it, you'll be the first to know. The Congress when they passed the law earlier, said there 20 needs to be a higher education commission on accessible instructional materials for the college level crowd. Your folks, and the Department of Ed 25 called us two weeks ago and 27 1 said, Congress wants us to make a higher education commission. We want you to lead it, that means they really want skip to 5 lead it, and so there will be a higher education commission doing what this law did for K-12, so I know that skip -- thank you, thank you. 10 >> >>: skip, I know that skip, skip and chuck have a view that it will be different than the way it works in K-12, so maybe 15 he'll talk about that tomorrow. >> >>: but print disability is what I want to focus on here. So I want to -- as I said, I 20 think this language is a watershed, and I think -- I'm sorry I forgot to say one other thing I want to do today. Largely I'm going to do things 25 that I often do but in each case 28 1 I'm going to say something new today about what I think is next, because the UVM people hope that I would talk a little 5 bit more about what's next. So in each case I'm going to do that. >> >>: and here, though, I 10 wanted to say that the -- what's next will be an elaboration of some of these concepts, but the critical thing was this term from seeing kids as having 15 learning disabilities to seeing them as having print disabilities. There's a very fundamental shift that I think is a HARBINGER of very important 20 things to come and I guess I want to say before we get there, that the difference is because it starts to define disability in a modern way, which is to say 25 a disability always occurs in 29 1 the interaction between an individual and their environment, disabilities are never decontextualized. That 5 is, you can all think of places where you are disabled and where you are in other respects would have been disabled but you're not, and trying to think, so the 10 world will start seeing it this way: I'm a little, have a little fetish for reading books and movies about climbing Mount Everest, something I would 15 never, ever, ever do, but it's just, you know, I'm sure you all have these little fantasy lives, so I read things about climbing Everest. One of the things 20 that's different about Everest is it's not actually a difficult climate the top, it's not treacherous, but there's almost no oxygen, so the real, as you 25 probably know, a quarter of the 30 1 people that have summited Everest have died, so you're putting yourself in this environment where there is just 5 not enough oxygen and then you're going to have to do some very, very hard things. So that combination, the environment does not have enough oxygen, and 10 you're being challenged to do your strongest work, makes most people disabled. Most people become disabled near the top of Everest and can't go to the very 15 top, and thousands of people have been within visual sight of the top and not make it, OK? And everybody knows that. Parties will begin with 60 20 people, and three will summit. Gigantic disability at the top. So that's a kind of a way to think about disability that in this room, you know, 97 of us 25 would be disabled at that moment 31 1 on Mount Everest, and then it makes you think, oh, well, then that's an odd way to think about it, but most people would be 5 disabled, that is, you would have lung conditions at that height. In fact, you get neurological conditions. Many people are unable to tell what 10 the right thing to do next is because there's just not enough oxygen for their nervous system to operate well. So we can make conditions in which all of us 15 will look disabled, OK? Mount Everest is probably too extreme an example. We'll have other ones as we go along. Most people look disabled at Mount 20 Everest at this altitude, most of us don't feel the neurological and the breathing disabilities we would feel out on Mount Everest, OK? 25 32 1 >> >>: the way that print disability changes things is in the same way. It says that the environment is part of what will 5 define whether you're disabled or not. Print is part of the definition of disability. That colocation, it's not just in the kid. Whether you're disabled or 10 not depends on the environmental conditions you're going to be in. In a print environment, many kids are disabled that are not disabled in the same way as 15 Everest, not disabled in other learning environments. So I've always hated and I'm sure skip does, too, calling kids learning disabled. When in fact the 20 learning conditions have been ones in which there's not enough oxygen. So saying to, you know, Hillary on the top of Mount Everest, you have a breathing 25 disability because you're so 33 1 bent over and strained, you know, is a nutty way of to think of it rather than thinking there's not enough oxygen here 5 for almost everybody, the fact that you're breathing at all is amazing. So print disability, that colocation, saying print is part of the problem, it's a 10 fundamental shift, what I think we'll see to say what's next is we'll talk about things that are curriculum-based disabilities. So print is just an example of a 15 curricular materials, but as we move forward and universal design moves forward, I think we'll start to talk about, does this child have a 20 curriculum-based disability meaning there's not enough oxygen in this curriculum for this kid. Then you can think about the solutions. The 25 curriculum is part of the 34 1 problem, though, and it makes you focus on the curriculum first, which is what we need to to. the problem of focusing on 5 the kid first is that it gets us into some bad loops. >> >>: OK, so who has a print disability? It's critical, 10 because only if you have a print disability are you entitled to the benefits of the NIMAS legislation, so this is the law as it talks about it, relates to 15 a really, really old law and it says it doesn't actually define print disability, which is really interesting, and when all this was happening, some 20 staffers would call us from time to time, for advice about how the law would be written. They never tell you that, they just very generally saying, if you 25 were going to talk about print 35 1 disability, would it be -- do you think it would be smart if we used the following, whatever? OK, it's really these kind of 5 interesting as-if conversations. But they didn't end up defining print disability, which we thought was sort of odd, that if you're going to have a law, you 10 can just say that kids have print disabilities and they have certain rights. You'd think you'd define it but they didn't. In fact, they said it's actually 15 often better to leave things in the law vague and let case study, case law, defining it over time and they said don't get uptight about it. That 20 actually law often works this way. It's sort of an organic view of and sometimes if you overdefine it you're hurting yourself. So they said we're 25 just going to go back and look 36 1 at a really, really old law, and skip knows a lot more about this than I do, but an old law, which before the library of Congress, 5 which defined four groups. Blind people, everybody gets it. That print materials for a blind person are not going to work. Persons who have visual 10 disabilities aren't blind, but have significant vision disability. Everybody gets it, OK, well a printed book has a specific font size so there's 15 going to be lots of people who can't use that. Third, persons certified by competent authority as unable to read or unable to use standard printed materials 20 as a result of a physical limitation, and for reasons that are quite curious, this word physical limitation has been expanded in some views to 25 include it even being the source 37 1 for students who have learning disabilities. They have a physical limitation we'll come to that when we talk about the 5 neuroscience. >> >>: and lastly, person certified by a competent authority, and you can see where 10 right away this starts to go awry because you're thinking, well, who would be a competent authority? And in the old days, when the law was written, we're 15 going to talk about these changes in the old days, they really thought doctors would be the competent authority, OK? And you think, oh, my God, I 20 would not want my doctor to decide whether my kid had a justified learning disability or not, because E. doesn't know anything about it, but at any 25 rate, that's one of the 38 1 problems, a competent authority. We wouldn't define it the same as they did back in 1933 or whenever this happened. But 5 competent authorities having a reading disability resulting from organic dysfunction and of sufficient severity to prevent their reading printed material 10 in a normal manner. >> >>: don't like the word "normal "there, but at any rate. And that was thought to really 15 save everybody, because the present framers wanted to separate out kids that are just bad readers, don't even ask me why they might be bad readers, 20 but the publishers would have gone berserk if all bad readers would have access to these digital materials. This is free of charge, by the way. So the 25 publishers needed something, and 39 1 so this language was tried out to say, OK, competent authorities, got to say who it is, and it's got to come from 5 organic dysfunction. Something's got to be wrong with their brain. It can't be that they just didn't have good teachers or they're poor, or 10 English isn't their first language or those things. That would be way too far. They should have an organic dysfunction. Can everybody see 15 why that sort of people, this is a compromise and it was reached there? >> >>: so I'm going to go after 20 this at some length as we talk, and talk about what works and doesn't work about this definition in the light of modern cognitive neuroscience, 25 but any questions about the 40 1 laws? The law says if you have a print disability, you have access, your school must provide you with these new kinds of 5 materials, and in a timely fashion and all of that, and it's the law of the law in every State of the Union and as I said, higher education 10 commission is now going to look at how do we do something like this for colleges and universe it's and you can go after skip tomorrow about what's going to 15 happen. And don't tell too many people about that because it's not official. >> >>: I probably shouldn't have 20 said anything. >> >>: OK. So I want to talk about three advances that have happened since the time that 25 those four things were written. 41 1 And like I said, I'm not sure I made it clear, that there's no definition, but what the law says is if you're one of those, 5 you're OK. You can get NIMAS materials. So it's not really defined. It just says there's the kind of people who are print disabled. 10 >> >>: and so we're going to go through today three kinds of advances. And the culture in science that would make that law 15 be framed very differently now and described and I think is where the law will go. So first is the neurosciences. We learned a lot in the last seven 20 years about how the brain really works, how it learns and in particular about individual differences in kids. So that we wouldn't say things exactly the 25 same. 42 1 >> >>: first, the big change that we want to say is that now to a cognitive neuroscientist, neuroscientist, any kind of 5 neuroscientist, all learning abilities and disabilities are organic. They don't think there's any kind. They're all organic. So you're not going to 10 be able to use oh, this one is organic or it's not organic. So I want to talk a little bit about how learning works, OK? 15 >> >>: these are PET scans and you skip probably showed you a couple of these, and I think explain that the brighter it looks, the more in this case 20 glucose is being burned, so it's a way to sort of map the brain and look at what parts of the brain are most active. The more active it is, the brighter, the 25 more hot it looks like it's 43 1 burning. So that when you're seeing words, there's a very hot area there, a couple of others. When you're hearing words, very 5 hot area there and a little bit less, a little bit less in some areas that aren't very involved. And why glucose? Now we do oxygen, as well. What you're 10 doing is measuring every time a neuron fires, that takes energy so it's got to reuptake some more glucose, it's got to reuptake some oxygen so it can 15 fire again. So when neurons are firing, they're taking up glucose. So all they're doing is measuring how fast are these parts of the brain burning 20 glucose. So when you're hearing words, you're -- you tend to burn glucose here. When you're seeing the same words, you burn it in a slightly different 25 place. So this is visual 44 1 cortex, this is auditory cortex it, no big surprise. >> >>: I guess I want to go back 5 just to make my point. We'll see this in >> >>: question? 10 >> >>: is that the left or the right side of the brain? Into most of the slides I'll show will be the same. This will be the front and that's the back. 15 You can always tell the front because the temporal lobe points toward the front. >> >>: left or right? 20 >> >>: these will be -- this is so just picture if you were facing the front this would be your left side. So just look at 25 the temporal lobe right there, 45 1 this will always be visible. Just say that's pointing toward the front, OK? 5 >> >>: we'll have a little bit of cause to look at how different tasks can be. It's really cool, the last 20 years you've seen this explosion of 10 our ability to study how does the brain accomplish learning, and in specific for different kinds of tasks. Different kinds of work. But another thing 15 that's been amazing, and more recent, is we can actually study the brain not after it's left-hand, but while it's learned and watch what kind of 20 changes actually happen in the nervous system. And I like to do this among educateors. If you look here, this is a naive brain, just like we were looking 25 at before, and by the way, these 46 1 are college sophomores. They're almost always college sophomores because they are free and they're past the trauma of 5 freshman year and they're usually in their major now and so you can demand that they get in these experiments. So -- and this task, a very simple task, I 10 don't think I'll bother to explain what the task is, but here we have a task that lights up some areas here in the temporal cortex and a big area 15 in the frontal cortex, pretty hot in the middle. So this is a thought experiment for you. This is actually right away when they started doing the task, 20 it's very easy. You say a noun to the student and they're just supposed to say a verb. Not hard but you say car, they say drive. Tree, they say grow, 25 whatever? OK, all they have to 47 1 do is say a verb. And there's no right or wrong, but just keep within class verb. Turns out, though, that changes what the 5 brain does very significantly, just to even say a verb. But anyway, that's what it is. What I'd like you to think about is, after they get good at that, 10 that is, this is at the beginning. The brain's going to change as it learns, so how would it look here? After they've done it for half an 15 hour, it's actually shorter than that, what would change? How would this use of oxygen, of glucose here, how would it change? And it's interesting. 20 Neurologists, many neurologists guessed exactly wrong with p what would happen. And a lot of them just had the wrong idea of what would happen. Usually when 25 asked educators, educators could 48 1 guess correctly. So just take a moment and see if you're more like a neurologist or an educator. What do you think the 5 brain is going to look like here? >> >>: OK, let me just take some hypotheses. Anyone -- I need to 10 get a little bit closer. Anyone willing to hazard. If you know, don't raise your hand. >> >>: with practice it might 15 get better, more efficient, so maybe use less of the brain? >> >>: great. The hypothesis as you practice, it would get 20 better, more efficient, so it would burn less glucose. Another hypothesis? >> >>: may move to a different 25 area. 49 1 >> >>: it could move to a different area because maybe the task looks different when you gain expertise and you approach 5 it differently. Mm-hm. Any others? >> >>: now, you haven't said what the neurologists thought 10 might happen. So none of you are neurologists, apparently. And actually, both of you are right but the neurologists thought maybe the brain would 15 become more involved, like it would just be what gets smart is that the brain becomes more and more holistically involved. But actually proven again that 20 educators are smarter than neurologists. In fact, exactly what you said happened which is that it gets very efficient in it and you have a great 25 reduction in the glucose burn. 50 1 But we're only showing part of the brain and in fact, among other mings just to give you a little example, most tasks when 5 you begin them, you tend to use the right side of your brain much more heavily, and as you get more and more expertise, it moves much more focally to the 10 left side and then begins to get much more tightly coupled with specific areas, so you actually do burn less glucose in those areas when you become practiced, 15 and it changes often, because you start treating the task quite differently, different parts of the brain light up. OK? Everybody with me? And the 20 novel is all of a sudden you come back and you say I have a got some new words I want to try and the brain lights up and says U owe uh-oh. I've got something 25 new to learn here. 51 1 >> >>: I like to talk about this, and Charlie set me up nicely to connecting to VIKOTSKY that in fact the glucose burn is 5 much closer to what we actually want in learning is that we want kids to be burning glucose. Because it turns out that that burning of glucose and oxygen is 10 in fact the brain changing itself. It's becoming a different brain. It's a brain that knows how it to do this now and you can actually watch it 15 wire itself. And sculpt itself into being a brain that does this task. And typically leashing locks like that, but it doesn't change itself under two 20 conditions. One is, whoops, that's a new button pusher here, one is that you already knew how to do that. In which case the brain doesn't burn any glucose 25 and doesn't change itself, no 52 1 learning is going to occur and the other, and you won't be surprised at this either, is if the task is too hard, if the 5 task is too hard for you to change your brain in order to be able to do it, in fact a similar thing happens, you don't in fact burn glucose, either. So 10 VIGOTSKY talked about being in the zone of proximal development. That you've got to learn just enough to burn enough glucose. Too hard you aren't 15 going to change and too easy you aren't going to change. And the hard part is how to do it with a whole lot of people that come in to the class class at very 20 different places. How can I be just hard enough for 25 students who range from people who know a lot to people who know very little and et cetera. 25 53 1 >> >>: a second point I want to make, people are different. And when we look at learning in the nervous system, we can see that 5 they don't learn alike, and I have a collection of things. I'm not going to bother to go through them now, but one of the things that I would ask you to 10 look at is you'll often say brain image slides of the changes that occur in learning now. But what's often left off, except in the methodology 15 section, is that they're aggregates, they're averages. Well, we put all the sophomores together. Because they've excluded all the people who 20 might be a little bit weird, all the people who are too bright, all the nonEnglish speakers, all the left handers so they've taken out all the people who 25 might be different and then 54 1 aggregated everybody who's left and say well, this must be average but what's interesting and if you talk to a 5 neuroscientist they'll say it's true. If you look at individual slides you'll see great variability. They don't all look the same. It's just that 10 wage them so you can kind of make a general point, OK? But they really look quite different. You've probably seen and heard of these examples, but 15 this is SHAYWITZ's work that's very familiar to probably lots of you. This is Newsweek putting the word normal here. So a typical reader tends to 20 show areas that light up in the posterior, the back part of your brain, and typically areas sort of threeish of them that look like that. And when you look at 25 and this is reading single 55 1 words. If you look at a dyslexic, those areas aren't lighting up. They're not being used. The brain has not 5 sculpted itself to read with those parts. It's actually reading with this part. What I'd like to show to you is there's been a great expansion 10 somewhere else. It's not that the brain isn't trying. But it's trying with a different part of the brain, OK? A differentiated part. It's 15 reading with frontal cortex, this is reading largely with these areas in posterior cortex. We'll have reason to understand this a little bit later, but and 20 now you probably know there's dozens of experiments going on where people are doing early interventions with reading and looking to see if you succeed in 25 your early interventions, do 56 1 dyslexic readers start to have normal patterns and lots of them do. So we did our early interventions, we worked on them 5 and in fact their brain starts to light up more typically. Not all do, but some do. >> >>: here's another one that 10 makes the point really about not differences between people, but differences within you, within a single person, that changes over time. When teenagers are shown 15 emotional faces, they're trying to read the emotion in faces or voices or things like that, what part of the brain do they use? Sorry, I clicked the wrong 20 thing. >> >>: they tend to, what lights up most is the amig dulla, which is a very old phylogenetic 25 structure. Lots of really 57 1 stupid animals have amig dullas, and for a lot of animals we think we're really smart because we've got this neural cortex all 5 over the place that we have more processing capacity than animals that have an amig dalla have. So anyway, teenagers the largest chink that happens when they're 10 learning emotional information. Adults, on the other hand, the thing that lights up most, where you would burn most glucose is in free frontal cortex, orbital 15 frontal, prefrontal cortex up in the front of the brain: And rely more on the cortex. Which differentiates us from most animals, and most particularly 20 monkeys and most particularly us. So you have to think about a teenager is actually processing the information with a different brain than you have. 25 Because this part of the brain 58 1 is very late developing. It develops all the way through adolescence. If you look at it physiologically and if you look 5 at it anatomically, it's still an immature part of the brain. So teenagers do the best they can with the parts that are mature. You are using a really 10 a different brain. So when you think how come he couldn't tell I was sarcastic, you've got to realize, oh, my God, he was just using his aMYGDALA, it doesn't 15 understand schasm. You need a lot of cortex to understand sarcasm and scorn. So our brains change over time and sometimes we ask kids to do 20 things that they don't have the brain yet to do. And our brains, probably you've seen this, this is the kind of stuff that's on NPR, and various 25 shows, that in the last five, 59 1 maybe a little bit more than that now, people have been able to watch the brain change as a result of experience. So the 5 first thing I did was the dyslexic is different than typically achieving. A young person is different than an old person. But also a person 10 that's had experience has a different brain than a person that doesn't have experience. And the classic one that sort of rocked everybody's boat was 15 studying taxicab drivers in England, and in particular, looking at the hippocampus, which was the structure I was really interested in when I was 20 in graduate school and they found out that the hippocampus in taxicab drivers in London was much bigger than regular people. And it was a shock, because 25 obviously they didn't learn to 60 1 drive a taxicab until they were adults. So everybody's assumed that well, your brain could change during childhood with 5 your experiences, but hey, 30 years old, people weren't expecting to see physically the brain look different, look much bigger in some places just 10 because they drove taxicabs. Now this has been done lots of times, so in fact the experiences we have, even as adults, are changing our brain 15 from one kind of thing to another, from a brain that has a small hippocampus relatively speaking to a brain that has a much bigger one. And I think 20 I'll skip this. As you can make it go either way, and the -- I just want to see how I'm doing in time. I don't want to open up this video. I have a video 25 here but I can tell you the 61 1 results. That another thing that's very disturbing and important for us as educators, another experiential thing can 5 happen to that same area of the hippocampus. So if you're a taxicab driver. By the way, hippocampus is very important for spatial, what a shock, so if 10 you're going to do a lot of spatial locating yourself around the universe, that part of the brain says hey, we need to get bigger and stronger to do this. 15 And by the way they've been able to show that it's not just people with big hippocampus go into being taxicab drivers, it's really that you in in fact, it 20 does get better with experience. But the reverse can happen. Very disturbing work shows that if you stress, if you put an individual in stress, typically 25 these have been done with 62 1 children, but many with adult rats and monkeys and so on, that that hippocampus will have rink, physically shrink. So we're not 5 talking about subtle changes, we're talking about macroscopic things. It looks bigger in taxicab drivers and it looks smaller p if people that have 10 stresses over long periods of time. Traumas of orphanages, rape, whatever. The hippocampus gets smaller. So that means that whatever the hippocampus 15 does, it can get better with experience and it can get worse with some kinds of experiences, as well. Worse meaning not as functional, OK? 20 >> >>: and Charlie asked me to mention, I'll just trying -- I want to make sure because I'm going to go out of the order of 25 my slides. Oh, no, this is a 63 1 good place for what's next. So I want to tell you about some research that one of my own graduate students has just 5 completed for her doctorate, and I think it's going to be a knockout when it comes out, OK? And it's related to this. And it's related to your work. What 10 she wanted to study was -- this is the right time to talk about it, OK? This is a really cool new direction, and I need to say how to think about this. The 15 word is stress. And stress can be either good or bad. People who studied the nervous system discover that what would be a stress? A novel environment. 20 Something new and strange is a stressor. Your nervous system reacts and it gets prepared, OK? And it can go two ways. A frightening thing would do it, 25 too, but even just a novel new 64 1 thing is a stressor to the nervous system, you can see it mobilize. It goes in two directions. It can mobilize 5 positively like here's something I'm going to have to do or something I'm going to have to learn or something I'm going to have to be skillful about, and 10 people call that challenge. OK? So you can be stressed by challenge. Here's something I'm going to have to do. I'm going to have to have get better. I'm 15 going to have to get a better grade or whatever it is, OK? And your brain is mobilized. You can see itologically in your skin, in your eyes, all of these 20 things, go oh, get ready and we're going to change. The other way it can go, if the first thing is challenge, which mobilizes you, your brain to do 25 it, to learn new things, the 65 1 other is threat. In which case your brain mobilizes, not to learn new things, but to get out of here. OK? And that's your 5 basic flight or fright thing that you've heard many times. By you can see the brain mobilize in a different way and physiologically measure it in 10 your skin, your heart, your lungs, different things, threat is a different thing and threat is not a mobilization to learn stuff it's we need to get out of 15 here, this is a bad situation, OK? And the work that Same is doing comes from a realization that how does a certain decide whether it's going to be a 20 challenge, meaning mobilize to overcome this, versus a threat? And they have this really need equation they've realized that people do. People immediately 25 do an attribution which involves 66 1 how hard is it, how frightening it is it, how new is it, whatever it is. And the second piece they immediately assay is 5 what resources do I have to meet this stressor. It if you feel you have the resources and it may be an illusion that you have them, but if you feel you have 10 the resources, I think I could do that, I could do that with a shovel, I could do that if I had a little help, whatever, if you feel you have the resources, you 15 move toward challenge. If you feel you don't have the resources, this is too hard, I don't have any help, I don't have any tools, you move toward 20 threat and your body says, OK, this is not a good situation, get out of here. All right, is everybody with that distinction? So it's a beautiful, nice, all 25 stressors are going to lead 67 1 toward either challenge or threat and it depends on what resources you feel you have, and you can all remember yourself 5 feeling like that, some days you know you have more resources, I can handle this today. And other days you know I can't do it today. I don't have the 10 resources. Your body, your brain is making that calculation. >> >>: so what she studied is we 15 have these new kinds of readers that we'll probably show you pa little bit of later that give extra resources, extra support, so she wanted to study, well, if 20 you have extra support in the environment, does the stress, does the threat stress go down? So she thought what if I could do is get students with learning 25 disabilities, who would probably 68 1 be stressed toward the threat end, by giving a reading task, but what if I give them more resources, give them electronic 5 reader with the extra stuff that we've built in that you've probably seen some of and certainly we'll see more more of, does it move away from the 10 threat and toward the challenge and she got no results, and she was in despair, because this was a year's work and she's measuring LD kids and regular 15 kids to see if there's a difference, and nothing shows significant results. Until she actually looks at their stress level. She's measuring things 20 like glucose -- not glucose, what's the word I'm trying to think of cortisol, thank you, cortisol in their spit and their skin conductants, heart rates, 25 things like that, sophisticated 69 1 measures, so the kids are wired up. And then she does a different analysis and this is the part that's the knockout. 5 What she finds is that when LD kids, kids who had been identified in school, came into the situation, just coming into where she's sitting, a nice 10 quiet little room, nothing really bad in there and she says we're going to do a little reading, their tonic level of stress is, before she starts the 15 experiment, is .0001 different from the typically achieving kids. So they come in, the minute she says we're going to do a reading thing, they're at a 20 high stress and the typically reading kids are at a completely different stress level. So at beginning of they are experiment there are two different kinds of 25 brains. Some kids are in a 70 1 brain that is in a threat situation looking to how do I get out of this, and typically achieving kids are in a 5 challenge situation, cool, this is something that I might be able to do. So they didn't begin her experiment the same kids at all. They began as very 10 different kids. Some of them under -- both of them stressed. Some under threat and some under challenge and then she realized oh, my God, they're walking 15 around school like that they're chronically in a state of high stress. High threat stress. And she's going to start -- she just joined us at cast and she's 20 going to start doing studies where she's literally monitoring the walking around, because can you imagine what it would do when that gets out to national 25 television, that kids with 71 1 reading disabilities are walking around at stress levels that are pathological, and remember I just told you, that's doing bad 5 things to their hippocampus, among other things. So they're walking around in a pathologically, what's the word, preparatory pathological state, 10 too much stress. Just a knockout. So I think we'll coming come to understand that we're putting kids in situations which make them very different 15 and these kids are walking around, looking to how to get out of here, which is the way we experience while other kids are walking around with great sense 20 of challenge. >> >>: OK, I want to go back to individual differences. And I want to push the point that we 25 really do see things 72 1 differently. And everyone that has a spouse sort of knows that, but I want to just give you an example of how deeply it can be 5 there. So this is an experiment. The task is merely to draw this. But there's two groups of students. Students with Williams syndrome and 10 students with down syndrome. They're matched for IQ. OK, 50ish IQ, so these are kids that are down in the spectrum, all of them had intellectual 15 disabilities and you present the task and you you say will you draw what you saw and again I'm stressing same cognitive level. If IQ tests were that was 20 differentiating, no difference. But here's what they draw. The kids with Williams syndrome see detail. They see that this is actually composed of a lot of 25 little Y the and they draw all 73 1 the rel little Y's. The kids with dawn syndrome see this as a large shape, a D. Very different and the reason I'm 5 just using this, is to say that we often think that when we show kids the same thing or lecture the same thing, that everybody's got the same information. And I 10 want to say that that never happens. That that interaction that I talked about the at the beginning is where the knowledge will be constructed. And what 15 the kids with Williams syndrome matched in IQ with the down syndrome are seeing and remembering is very different when we presented the exact same 20 thing and we'll talk about when I talk about my class, I always think that everybody heard the same lecture, but if I look at their notes, they don't hear the 25 same lecture at all. OK? 74 1 >> >>: learning is also differentiated by task. I've talked about just finished individual differences, it looks 5 different. When I look at a task analysis, here's the brain and they're all still facing the same way. But with different tasks. And you can see and this 10 was the thing that has caused more of the cognitive science revolution than anything else. That the brain lights up very differently for viewing words 15 than for listening to words, than for speaking words, and than for generating verbs. All of these are language tasks, and the neuroscientists were like, 20 holy cow, it blows away the idea that there is he' some kind of large capacity called language, because in fact there's actually many components to language and 25 the brain treats these aspects 75 1 of language as very different things, and this is done now countless times, that looking at the brain physiology we've come 5 to understand that things that we thought were sort of the same or one kind of learning, the more you look at t the brain says that's not true at all. 10 These are very different things in the brain and look at the difference here, this is the one that the neuroscientists went, holy cow, this says any word 15 that you want to. This is what lights up. This one is that exact task I talked about a little bit ago. I say a noun and you say a verb. The change 20 from saying any word you want, to you got to say a verb, made the brain go completely different and see the task as very, very different. And now 25 we understand that verbs are not 76 1 treated in the brain in the same way that nouns are. Nobody knew that. And all sorts of individual difference about 5 language and all sorts of things can happen but in fact just saying I only want you to say verbs means the brain organizes itself differently, burns 10 glucose differently, acts differently and acts in fact differently. >> >>: would that issue be the 15 same if someone could say any word that they wanted to? >> >>: whoa p spoken like a neuroscientist. Would that be 20 different if the -- >> >>: can you repeat the question? If they were able to say any verb they wanted to, 25 would it change this so it would 77 1 look like this one. It was a great question, and they didn't do it. So the answer is, partly, but if in fact, one of 5 the tests of frontal lobe function, which is this, is to say something like say only words that fin with F or only words that are animal names or 10 only words that are verbs. It does change. So that you have to use this part of the brain to do that, as opposed to anything. OK? But it's a wonderful 15 question, because in fact, it would be still closer, though, than, the confrontation device of saying a noun and you've got to respond specifically to it is 20 what lights up this part of the brain, that is, you have to hear that first word. So it would look much more like the individual. That was very good. 25 So you should have been a 78 1 neurologist. >> >>: OK, and skip I think talked about this so I'm going 5 to breeze lightly for universal design for learning, we divide up the brain into three large systems. There's lots of ways to divide up the brain and this 10 one is a common way to think about it. Recognition networks in the back of the brains, teaching networks p in the front and affective networks in the 15 middle of the brain and they're going to help us orient to how do we think about learning and its differences? The recognition networks, the back 20 part of would your brain and it's very consistent, information that comes into your brain, always goes to the back part of your brain, whether it's 25 in your spinal cord or if it's 79 1 in your thalamus, anywhere, cortex, if the information is coming in, it goes to the back of your brain and with the tack 5 of cortex, high levels of -- sorry. With this part of the brain, all that part of the brain is designed to extract what is that that was on your 10 retina what is it that a hit your eardrum, et cetera, OK? And when you look at this image, that back part of your cortex lights up right away to say, 15 whoa, OK, what was that pattern of stimulation that hit me on the retina? Skip might have done that. One thing that's come out new that I want you to 20 know about is in the category of what's next. >> >>: some findings that have really excited neuroscientists 25 is well, how do you remember 80 1 things? OK, so to go back, if you're going to look at that picture, you're going to understand it here, back part of 5 the brain. Make sense of it, know that this picture works on all of those things. How do you remember it? Now, if I say to you, if I just don't look at 10 that slide for a minute, if I say remember that picture, OK, just do it for a second, that picture I showed you for a second, try to remember it, OK? 15 By the way, some of you will be fabulous at that, some of you will be terrible, which I don't have time to go into, but and but there's individual 20 differences but anyway, most of you will do OK. So the question is where is the memory? And in the last, in this last decade we've realized you you know what 25 you're doing when you remember 81 1 that? All you're doing is you're making -- I have to go back far enough to get to it. Here's visual cortex, that 5 lights up when you look at that picture, and you know what happens when you remember that picture? It lights up again. You are recreating, so the word 10 remember, remake it, you are remaking it in your brain. That's what you're doing. You say do that again. You literally tell your brain to do 15 it, and people have now, in the last couple of years it's really been just an explosion of this, so it will be the they'll literally put a little electrode 20 next to a neuron and they'll show you some pictures. Here's a picture, this picture, nothing. This picture, nothing. Simpson's, bing, OK? That's 25 like some of you. Goes crazy. 82 1 And that later they say, can you remember that third thing we showed you? What was that like? All right or I'm sorry they said 5 the real thing, Hollywood sign, nothing, the simple sons, boom, same cells. Go crazy. So you actually make the same cells light up again. The very cells 10 that you used to perceive it are the cells that you use to remember it. You just make them do it again. So you are remembering it, remaking it 15 again inside your brain. Isn't that cool? People didn't know that that was the case. >> >>: does that mean does that 20 mean we have Simpson cells? >> >>: no, because you have to excite millions of neurons. The fact that they had it next to 25 one, doesn't mean that that one 83 1 coded simp sons, but it meant that that was part of a large system of neurons that fired together to tell you that was 5 the simp sons. But now, people are going crazy watching people remake things that they saw before. By the way, where do you think you dream? 10 >> >>: there's only one visual place. When you dream, and you're having a big dream of either me or Brad Pitt, you, if 15 your dream has gone visual, this will light up again. The same part with which you recognize Brad Pitt's picture will be the part that you say, in the middle 20 of your night says make that for me again. Good old Brad, bring Brad Pitt back. Now, it's important to know that it works that way, because it means you 25 are constructing it. You're 84 1 making it. There's no real film back there. It's not locked in. It is an act of cognition. You are saying, I want to do that 5 again. The thing I did when I saw Brad Pitt live or on the movie, I want to make that again. So that's why eyewitness testimony is so bad. Because in 10 fact it always is a reconstruction and the more neuroscientists look at the way it works, the more they think oh, my goodness we shouldn't be 15 doing this eyewitness thing. Because it is always a cognitive act of reconstruction. There's no vault. There's nothing stored away. You make your 20 neurons do the same thing and of course you're never perfect and if somebody has given you a little information it in between when you remake it, you may make 25 him a little bit I think you've 85 1 heard all these experiments that just give you a little bit of information say didn't you like Brad Pitt's moustache? And then 5 you come in back in and remember Brad Pitt, you remember him with a moustache. We're able to distort because we're able to get in the way of you 10 reconstructing it, you making it again. OK, is everybody with me? So that's one of the things because and I'll come to you in one second, the enormous 15 individual differences in kids' ability to make those things, and the reason I paused there was that they've just done a study of kids' reading, and 20 they've shown that if it's high visual imagery reading and you're a good reader, visual cortex lights up like crazy in exactly the places you would 25 imagine that. I'm sorry, you 86 1 would perceive that so if it's about a purple Dragon, the very cells that would code purple if you look at them, start firing 5 like crazy, and what we can see is which kids are able to imagine what they're reading, because they're essentially remaking it inside their brains. 10 But some kids are not. Some kids are not doing that at all. They're not reimagining it and you can watch it in their brains and see it's not happening. 15 >> >>: go ahead, question. >> >>: well, this might take us too far afield, but your verbs 20 make -- where is making located in the brain? >> >>: I want to get there. Can we go just a little bit further? 25 Because you're right. It sounds 87 1 like is there a person in there? >> >>: exactly. OK, so when I use the word make, who's making 5 it? But I want you to get that sense of it's a construction. OK? It is not a -- the next thing and it's been coming for now a few years, is the 10 realization that there is no box, there's nothing stored in memory. It's because it's that you become better and better at reconstructing things and in 15 some cases, in a lot of cases you're constructing them anew, OK? But it if you give me one more round, then we'll come back to it, OK? And it's a good 20 question. >> >>: so but I've just talked briefly about about this part of the brain that allows you to 25 perceive the world, to recreate 88 1 it, to even dream it. This is your construction. And I realize you know what I wanted to say, is that in fact, it's 5 not ever a picture, even when you perceive it. It is something that you are constructing. And that would take a longer argument, but the 10 neuroscientists, the more they look at it, they realize you're not receiving. You're not receiving a picture. You are actually making up stuff on the 15 basis of what happens on your retina. And the more they look at it, the closer they look, they realize oh, my gosh, the brain is making it up. And 20 things like illusions are the dead give aways to see that, oh, my gosh you're seeing things that aren't there but those are just the tell tales that show us 25 it doesn't receive information. 89 1 It's making it up if if it's working well and so the dreams are like that. 5 >> >>: you do you mean real time or -- >> >>: real time. 10 >> >>: yeah, so for example, and -- >> >>: repeat the question? 15 >> >>: oh, real time are you making it up? Yeah. And I'll just give you an example so you can get a feel for what the neuroscientists look at. The 20 color orange. So you can all picture, the color orange feels like it's out there but in fact there's a whole book called the -- by ZEKI if you want to 25 read just a dramatic thing, he 90 1 looks at how color is perceived by the brain. Whole book on it t fabulous, DEKI and you know what he comes up? You don't 5 actually perceive color, you make it up and how you make it up is you take in a ton of information because what he shows us is that orange is not 10 stable, that in different lighting conditions it changes drastically what your retina does and what your THALAMUS does and even what your cortex does. 15 What your brain says is I know it doesn't have the same angstroms as orange but in this light it is orange. But your eye perceives two things that 20 are not orange. Your brain says, oh, that's just a lighting effect and it cancels it out, does it beautifully for you. I know I went too fast but I don't 25 want to stick here long enough. 91 1 So you make up orange and you know why that works? You can see why evolution did it, excuse the expression. All of your 5 ancestors that thought yellow was a specific thing on your retina are dead because when the lion was in less light, it said, oh, it looks like a lion but 10 it's not yellow. I wonder what it is. Those people all died. And the people who survived said, it's yellow, even though it doesn't look yellow. And 15 that's a lionment OK? And you do that all the time. >> >>: we did the Koffka ring yesterday. 20 >> >>: OK. Front part of the brain. Different networks. They allow you to plan, organize and initiate purposeful actions 25 on the environment. 92 1 >> >>: now, to your connection where's the maker, I would actually change this slide. Plan, organize and initiate 5 purposeful actions on the environment or the rest of the brain. In a way that we'll come to, OK? 10 >> >>: so when you looked at this picture, did you do this? >> >>: yeah, a little bit but there's people here who have not 15 seen it, either. >> >>: so when you looked at that picture, the front part of your brain made a plan. In a 20 half a second for how it is you're going to look at that picture, and people have studied this, and these are recordings to see what did you look at. 25 And we've done some of these 93 1 studies, too, at cast. And these are a plan, these are a strategy for how to look at that picture that sampled 60 times a 5 second. And here's a bunch more. And the question is, wow, why are they so different because these are the same picture, each time, a very 10 different plan or strategy to open your eyes to what makes them so different and just because I want to go quickly, I won't pause for the question of, 15 this is actually the same person looking seven times at that picture. Why so different? And the answer is, because a different question was asked. 20 In a half a second and now we can watch the brain mobilize, challenge to say, oh, given that question, this is how I would look, including like for example 25 how are the people in this room 94 1 related and you go boom, boom being, boom, boom, skipping everything else and if I ask you something like, is there a cat 5 in the room, you go all over the place, it could be anywhere. And that planful ability to make intentional acts is what the front part of the brain allows 10 you to do. OK, to make a good plan and to execute it. And if we cut off the very front of your brain, this is the same task and ask you different 15 questions, you actually don't have a different plan. You do the same, no matter what question I ask, if you have prefront quail damage, you look 20 at the picture the same way. That ability to be planful, strategic about how you look at a picture is part of the same cortex that grows late in the 25 adolescent. It's a very 95 1 late-developing structure, so young children and lots of adults, are not mature in the way that they can be strategic 5 about how they get information. Whether it's from text or an image, a image is sort of dramatic but the same is true of text. So they actually don't 10 differentiate what they do by the question. They don't have a purpose driving. You know what's driving? The outside world is driving. They're 15 reactive, responsive to the outside world, whereas people who are affective adults are strategic in dominating the world. That is, they go after 20 it and say I'm here for a purpose and you have lots of students who don't read for purpose. They don't know why they're reading the text, 25 they're just working their way 96 1 through it and you are hoping, I'm hoping they're reading this to get what I want out of it and a lot of them aren't. And some 5 actually would have great difficulty doing that. >> >>: watching ciz as they actually look at a textbook, so 10 here's the first second and a half, I think, you can see what the person looks at first. And what these studies showed us was that in fact, if you really 15 trace the eye movements of students who have quote-unquote reading disabilities and we replay this like a movie and show what did they look at and 20 you play it back in the order, it's incomprehensible. You find out that they never had a good strategy for how to look at this page. They were distracted by 25 all these images and things, 97 1 they didn't know where to go next. So it goes like this, it sort of jumps around and realize oh, my gosh if I gave them that 5 information in that order which is what's happening I wouldn't understand it. So the new kinds of textbooks are actually very problematic for kids that don't 10 have good executive functions in order to make their own plan. They don't know where to go and so it's really kind of a hopeless jumble. 15 >> >>: the last part of the brain, I'm going to come back and say what's next in that part in a minute. Oh, but I want to 20 go back to your question. So when we ask you to remember the simpsons, two things light up. >> >>: actually, three things as 25 we'll see in a minute. But for 98 1 the reasons I just talked about, visual cortex would light up, the very parts that allowed you to perceive it, the right 5 colors, the right shapes, all of that would light up. But also, free prefrontal cortex lights up.S executive that says this is what I'm after to recreate that. 10 It says make that image of the Simpson's again. So we'll see two things light U7 and this will light up and in fact I'll see if I can queue it up for the 15 afternoon. Have a wonderful time of seeing it actually happen. First you see the image, it jumps up to prefrontal cortex to get a plan and you'll 20 take that away and you'll see the prefrontal cortex first and then the visual cortex lights up. It's just fabulous stuff. So you have a plan that you make 25 that image and that's how you 99 1 remember it, so it's an intentional act of remembering. >> >>: OK. Much deeper than 5 probably you want to go. We'll get to this. >> >>: how did I get way back here? Ah. Oh, I'm going 10 backwards. I still haven't figured out this. Yeah, review part is too hard for me. The last and favorite part the is the middle part of your nervous 15 system which allows you to monitor the internal and external environment to set priorities and to motivate learning and behavior. This is 20 the critical thing, and people at cast are always angry that I don't start with this because this is what really is the center, and the nice thing is it 25 is in the center of your brain. 100 1 This is the center of who you are. It's the part of your brain that defines for you what's important. This above 5 all the other things happening in the universe, this is what you want to pay attention to. This is the food you're going to eat. This is who you're going 10 to look at. This is what you're going to be afraid of. What this does is color your experience and say, this is -- you experience it as feeling. 15 This is how I feel about it. But your nervous system is actually feeling about everything. It never is not doing that. It is always 20 looking -- when you look at something, this part of your nervous system goes to work immediately to figure out, of what importance is it? Not what 25 is it, which the back part of 101 1 your brain does, not what would I do with it or do to look at it which the front part of your brain does, but what importance 5 is it to me? Why do I care about this? And if I don't care, I'm going to move on. >> >>: and I've had some recent 10 experiences. I've really taught me a lot about this. When you -- the what next part. I think that people now realize, I know you're worried I'm going to 15 get closer and closer. But the -- when you walk around this university, if you've been here before, your nervous system is recognizing objects, which is a 20 construction, you say oh, yeah, yeah, I saw that before. I remember I did that and I can do it again. The front part of your brain, what's my strategy, 25 what am I trying to do here, 102 1 what's my plan, where am I going, all those things. This middle will part is sampling this entire university for spots 5 that are important, places where you have affect, where things that were important and mighting important might be important for you might happen again. You can 10 see evolutionarily why it's important. When you had a good meal, you are your nervous system says this is where I had a good meal. If you saw lion 15 tracks there before. Your nervous system says look at it more, run away from it, remember it. All of those things, this part of the nervous system is 20 saying, it's important. >> >>: and I'm going to el tell you just a personal anecdote that has taught me how powerful 25 this is, that we don't think of 103 1 ourselves as walking around in an emotional landscape. We picture a pictorial visual landscape and an auditory 5 landscape. If you shut your eyes you'll recognize where you are by the sounds and all those things, and similarly by an action landscape but you also 10 are moving in this emotional landscape and I don't know if this is the -- well, this will tie two together. So I had a surgery for cancer about six or 15 eight years ago, and had a surgery which damaged by bladder, OK? You don't care about this, right? There's a story there. So it so I'm 20 healed, I don't have cancer, it's cool, OK of the but I have scar tissue there. OK? And that scar tissue is muscle tissue, but if it crunches like 25 a you know, a scar that you have 104 1 on the skin, if it crunches it's painful, OK? Now I never knew this, but preptory to your going to the bathroom, you have a 5 little sphincter that tightens up so that you're preparing to go to the bathroom. It tightens so you can urinate. So when that happens to me, I have an 10 instant little sharp pain, OK? Because it's just the scar tissue is right there, so I'll get pain if I am about to go to the bathroom. But here's the 15 shocking -- I don't know if shocking is the right word, but I actually find I get that pain in lots of places, like every time I go to a gas station, I'll 20 get a pain in my bladder. Because my nervous system is going, hey, this is a good place to go to the bathroom. And it just alerts me. You know, this 25 is important. You've got to 105 1 keep track, David, sometimes you forget. So here it is. And if it's a regular place, like I get a little pain when I go by the 5 men's room in the university where my class is. Every time I go by. Whether I have to go to the bathroom or not I'll get this little blip and it's just 10 like my nervous system is going all the time I'm checking what are the important things there. Oh, there's that bathroom, Davy, you know you like that. And you 15 all know that there are parts of Cambridge all of a sudden you feel emotion coming over, you realize oh, my God that's when I was walking with Ruth when we 20 discovered she was pregnant or something and if we put the wires on you, you'd see that emotion. And you'd see your emotions going all over the 25 place as you're traveling 106 1 through Cambridge. Some parts are frightening and your nervous system would go oh, stay away from there, and over there is 5 where you had your first kiss or whatever and your nervous sis it tem is keeping track of this so you'll know the important places just like animals learn to avoid 10 some and go somewhere else. And I wanted to point that out just to say that so much happens that our nervous system is doing this we're unconscious of. I never 15 knew that my nervous system was keeping track of bathrooms until I had the scar tissue and your nervous system is keeping track of things like that and you feel 20 you've got all this free will and you're just saying I think I'd like to go to the bathroom down but actually your nervous system is saying hey, you've got 25 a little down there and this 107 1 would be a good time and you're going you know, I feel like I need to go to the bathroom and your students are sitting there 5 in the class and you think you're giving the most important point and their nervous systems are saying all kinds of things to them. So it mon does the 10 internal, do I got a lot of water in my bladder, is there to external environment, is there a bathroom nearby and learning and behavior, this is a good time to 15 go to the bathroom and when you look at this picture, the nervous system is calculating immediately what's important here? What do I care about here 20 and that varies by who you are, what's been happening to you. If you're pregnant you tend to look over here, it looks like a baby over here, child, if you're 25 angry, you say that good looks 108 1 angry and coming a at me and Rorschach's of course are a way to measure all of this. 5 >> >>: so when we do anything, these three networks are engaged. Almost anything. It's very hard to separate them in reality. And in fact, I just 10 realized we're back to your question about who makes it. When you remember something, the back part of your brain reconstructs it, and by the way, 15 not perfectly, like we said, front part of your brain is the one that's trying to choose what it is you're going to remake, and I hope you'll see, now, too, 20 that if I remake it, I'll also feel it. So if I reconstruct something in the past that had strong emotion, this part of my nervous system will say, oh, 25 part of your memory is the 109 1 feeling itself. So this part is tracking everything that's happening to you, everything literally, to judge whether it's 5 important or not, and when you remember it, that comes with it. Because they stay connected. So it goes, so when you dream, it's really cool. The visual stuff 10 is going crazy. And your emotions are going crazy and that's what you experience in your dreams. You are dreaming with both and we'll do one more 15 thing and we'll bring those together. But so people can have trouble with reading, because they have trouble recognizing the patterns of 20 reading because they don't have good strategies for reading, or pause they don't think the reading is important. Or in some cases as SAMMY's research 25 shows, SAMMY's kids have had 110 1 threats when they've read before. So their nervous system remembers it. So when they come in and you say. Don't worry 5 about it, this isn't going to count for your grades. Their nervous system is going to they can't stop it and you can say all you want to the kid, don't 10 worry, this isn't a big deal, you'll do fine, the nervous system is saying forget that, every time we do something like this, bad things have happened. 15 And it is powerful, very strong thing. OK. And this is probably a place that you it shall some of you may feel like you need to go to the bathroom, 20 so I'm a little bit behind, so how long do you usually take? Ten minutes, no longer than ten minutes, OK? Thanks ... ... ...: Test test test test test 25 test test test we're going to 111 1 begin again in one minute. Those of you in conversation and those of you in the hall, come on back. We have a little lost 5 and found department at the registration table. We have a VGA adapter that was left in the room yesterday, so if you're missing your VGA adapter for 10 your laptop check the registration table in the hall. OK, part two ... ...: >> >>: the preceding section was 15 just to say, I want to make a summary, actually ... the more we understand about what happens to the brain with learning, the more we realize that again, 20 going back to that original definition, organic, that in fact, the very act of learning is an organic change in the brain. It really isn't a way to 25 get around. You can't separate 112 1 things that are organic from things that aren't organic, that the taxicab drivers in London are making an organic change to 5 their brain. When your students are in your class, they're making an organic change to the brain. That's the way we remember things. Glucose and 10 the oxygen are burned to make those connections change, so all learning is an organic change in the brain and everything that we call disability is also an 15 organic change, so if I stress you, and you damage your hippocampus, it's an organic change. So the old view from seven years ago that you could 20 separate people into those that had some kind of organic damage and those that didn't have organic damage, to a neuroscientist it's impossible 25 to figure out. Right now in the 113 1 last hour you've made organic changes to your brain and all of you that drank coffee hey, you know, did a little damage. 5 >> >>: so that is a criterion for kids who should get NIMSA versions isn't going to hold up to where is this going, because 10 sooner or later there will be a big court case and they're going to trot in ouro scientists in and they're going to say hey, it's all organic. There are 15 brains that look entirely normal that are incredibly disabled to our present way of thinking and brains that are the very first neurological case that I saw was 20 called an orange rind case where most of the kid's brain were missing except for the outer surface and they were in college, you know, had a little 25 bit of a learning disability but 114 1 you know, we're doing fine with mostly no brain there so they're going to bring those slides in and they're going to say, OK, 5 here's organic brain damage of the severest kind and this kid is doing fine and here's a kid that looks perfectly normal in any way you could look at it and 10 they're having trouble learning something. There's not any slides that you could put up that say OK here's a kid that should get a nimas version. So 15 we wouldn't write it that way now and we have to think about and skip will probably talk about things like the market and stuff, what are we going to do 20 if that kind of distinction isn't criteria by which kids can have better books. All right, the second change that's happened in the last seven years 25 is our view of disability itself 115 1 has changed very dramatically. And in the old days, disability was something that resided in the individual. In 5 architecture, the movement for universal design began first, and this, too, began a change in our understanding of how to think about these things. So 10 Ron MACE, the architect introduced universal design to architecture, but by doing that, it started to subtly change. When you started to see the 15 building as part of a problem and part of the definition of who is disit abled, who is handicapped, inevitably a shift with only its slightest evidence 20 then began that we're going to run through. I'm going to do this very quickly but this is the kid who started us off on our work. Very physically 25 disabled, unable to move 116 1 anything but his eyes and his chin. And this, you know, we got him moving and communicating with his chin, because he can 5 use fabulous computers and do things. Without those, devices, he was bound for a profoundly retarded institution. And because he couldn't speak, he 10 couldn't hold up his head, he couldn't point, he couldn't talk or walk or and he just looked inert. But if we gave him a little switch on his chin that 15 went out to a computer, he was able to learn Morse code very quickly and we realized oh, my God, he could communicate and then he was able to learn to 20 drive a wheelchair, as skip remembers somewhat wreck lessly, but at any rate, he was actually mobile and all that and he's actually at Community College 25 now. But his disability changed 117 1 drastically when he had an output channel that could work for him and then we had to deal with the school which was meant 5 to be accessible but of course wasn't, and the fixing it up after is of course problematic, expensive, damaging, et cetera, all of those, so movement toward 10 universal design that says build a building right from the start and the louver has a nice combination of elevator and stairway. Providing 15 alternatives, but those alternatives change inevitably the view of who is disabled and who is not, but the building is part of that handicapping 20 condition, not just what a kid does, just like Mount Everest. >> >>: and what you are doing and I'm doing is part of a 25 change I think that's going to 118 1 be very dramatic. And that is a change away from seeing people as coming in a standard variety, and then unusual or marginal or 5 nominalist cases. And the more you look at the nervous system and this is what the disability movement said, is that people are always on a spectrum., a 10 wide spectrum, and what we need to do is look at what's the normal variation in humans? And the what do we need to pay attention to that people vary 15 on? This movement is -- how many people for how many people is the word neurodiversity a word in your language? Oh, not many. So this is a what's next 20 part of this, neurodiversity. There's two new books coming out this spring, maybe one came out already, on neurodiversity. New term. I think this will be a 25 very sticky thing, and it's huge 119 1 in some circles. And neurodiversity is saying that we all differ in a lot of interesting ways, and it's only 5 when confronted with a specific situation like school, like Mount Everest, like a building, that things get cut off as this is says you have a disability in 10 this environment. And the neurodiversity movement is about making sure we pay attention to what diversity really looks like so that the environment will not 15 be disabling, and so I'll give Charlie these slides so you can click on these things, but this is, I think, just a website you can quickly go to if you want to 20 see these arguments. There's lots of them. And this is an aggregator site, and if you type brain .HE you'll to it and what's the upside of the fact 25 that we're diverse? And what -- 120 1 you'll find here is some amazing things. This is not even the best site, necessarily, but I'm going to -- I want to play just 5 a moment, because here's someone I like listening to. >> >>: hi, my name is Emily. I am 25 years old, and I have 10 Asperger's syndrome. Um, I decided maybe since I have such a hard time talking to people about it, it might be a good idea to make videos about it on 15 the computer. So I'm going to. It's true. I guess the first thing I would like to tell you about Asperger's syndrome is what it is. Asperger's syndrome 20 is a mild form of autism. That means you're not exactly rain Rainman but for all intents and purposes out in the world, you seem a little odd. I was first 25 diagnosed officially with 121 1 Asperger's syndrome when I was 23, so I went through my entire childhood and my teenage years and the beginning of my 5 adulthood without actually having a name for this condition. I have a little brother, and he was actually diagnosed when he was about 12 10 or 13. We're eight years apart. So it was around the same time. Mostly we were just thought of as weird in school. We didn't pay attention very well, we 15 didn't make friends very easily, and concentrating was hard. Asperger's syndrome is different from everybody. 20 >> >>: she takes out a cigarette, you weren't expecting that, were you sh >> >>: I know for me, it 25 involves being unable to read 122 1 facial expressions easily and also being unable to present the proper facial expression. A lot of my friends say that when they 5 come up to me quickly and they have a piece of news, be it exciting or bad or happy, I have this sort of surprised, confused look on my face and one thing if 10 you know me and you know what's going on, it isn't a big problem, but when you're trying to make friends in school or network ... 15 >> >>: I'm sorry. She's wonderful. She's got a bunch of videos. I just want to introduce you to her for two 20 reasons. One is because there's this vibrant network of people with Asperger's. It's unbelievable. She's got 159,000 views and she's not the most 25 popular, actually. And people 123 1 with Asperger's and people in the autism spectrum have found, because of the new media, next topic we're going to, this 5 ability to communicate that just was not there and find each other, so it's been just an astonishing thing and I wanted mostly to show you so here's all 10 of these are people with awes autism. I don't know when that is. So don't look at that. The bikini ... But any way, these are mostly people with autism, 15 and look at this word, because this is the word that you'll here more and more, neurotypicals and the myth of Asperger's. So within the 20 Asperger's community, which is now a community, didn't used to exist, because they didn't have ways that they found each other, we're called -- well, I'm sorry, 25 I shouldn't even think, probably 124 1 many of you are not neurotypicals. But at any rate it's a derogatory term for people like you, neurotypicals 5 are said like oh, she's a neurotypical and that means oh, I get it, and that means too emotional, can't concentrate on you know, the substance of the 10 matter, not very good truth-tellers, they have all a long list of neurotypicals, and how the difficulties they have, and the kinds of jobs they can 15 do, they're good receptionists, but you wouldn't want to really hire them for an tural job, actuarial job and fabulous. And they're just charming, charming 20 people. But this will -- I think, thrive, as people begin to see the diversity as what's really there and the problem is when you confront standardizing 25 conditions, then some people, 125 1 various kinds, will get marginalized as nonappropriate. So the actually on my way up, I was hearing a story that I 5 wanted to tell you which I think some of you have heard on Simon BEHREN Cohen who does research on autism spectrum disorders, he's actually SASCHA's brother 10 or cousin. So anyway, one is a neuroscientist and the other is rich. Here's how Simon BEHREN Cohen got interested in. He's a cognitive scientist at Oxford 15 union university and he noticed that a lot of his colleagues had kids who had autism and he was thinking, wow, why so many? Is it in the water? So then he did 20 a study, how many do, and most of his colleagues were in computer science, in linguistic science and these kinds of things, and the incidence was 25 very high, and like a good 126 1 scientist, he thought, well, what about other departments? Is it just Oxford so he goes to you know, English, history, 5 things like that. Very few autistic kids. A lot of manic depressive kids in those families and he goes whoa, what is that about? Huge amount of 10 autism and Asperger's in these mathematics, science, et cetera, huge amount of depression, manic depression, et cetera, in all of the liberal arts. And that 15 began a long set of studies. The point of which is to say, and the radio show was about this, if you go to Microsoft, not to speak out of turn, an 20 enormous number of people and their children have Asperger's, OK? Seattle schools have special people come in because they have so many kids with 25 Asperger's and autism. And 127 1 Simon BEHREN Cohen thing is they're part of the spectrum and you're going to see inevitably people that are very good at 5 some things and not so good at others and it's no surprise to you to know that it's a huge overproportion of people who have emotional problems who are 10 writers and poets and artists and filmmakers and stuff. That that's part of the gift, as well as the disability. That this variation is a normal part of 15 the human condition, and that people with autism and Asperger's are part of a spectrum of which there's great strengths and I don't want to go 20 through a lot of examples. This is just a recent article I picked out to more to show you that there's hundreds coming out like this, which looks at the 25 structural brain differences 128 1 between kids with autism and kids without, but instead of looking for what's broken in their brains, it's looking for 5 what are their brains good at and what they find, in lots of fields now, is that of course their brains are very good, and I don't mean just very good for 10 someone who's disabled, I mean they're better than you. And they're finding this in all kinds of, one of our people that works with us at cast is just is 15 going to come out in science soon, science is the top of the runk kind rung kind of hard-core science journal showing that there's a. Astro physicists. 20 What they did was they looked at what are the abilities require in astrophysics and they did tests and they show that actually, yeah, they're not good 25 readers, but they're really good 129 1 at the things that an astro physicist needs to do and he didn't have any trouble recruiting subjects, sent out an 5 email. Do any of you you know, the famous astro physicist dyslexia, he was flooded with all these people who are astro physicists, not considered a 10 low-level occupation, overrecommendation. And they said you know what, to be an astro physicist, you need the kind of things that dyslexic 15 kids have. And similarly, I'll tell you one more example. I think what you're going to see is this is in the what next category you'll see thousands of 20 things like this. So another recent one came out, done by an neurologist who was taking my class, fabulous. There's this TMS it's called, transcranial 25 stimulation, you can give 130 1 someone a temporary lesion, you just put this little pad, it's it kind of gives them a jolt. It it's like giving them a 5 little modern electroshock to their brain. Sounds horrible, but for as long as it's applied, that part of the brain stops working. It's just like giving 10 a temporary lesion but then you stop and it goes back to normal. So it's kind of this new way to study what the different parts of our brain do. So he was 15 interested in autistic kids and in particular, autistic kids, if you throw out 100 things, roughly 100 things, and you have a lot of things on a table and 20 you say to people how many of them are there, you're terrible at it. You say something like I don't know, 37, 400, you don't have a clue and someone noticed 25 if you throw them down and you 131 1 have autistic kids, they say 87, 32 and they're right. And people went what, how could you do that? And they did it and 5 showed yup, they're really good. Not every autistic kid but this astonishing ability to recognize pneumo NUMEROSITY is a high-level thing and you know 10 what they found out if you take your brain and electrostimulate it in the right place, all of a sudden you can do it and they were like, what? So actually 15 inside of each of you is a really smart autistic brain that could do, you could be an astro physicist p you could be an actuarial table. It's just that 20 your brain is getting in the way of some of these incredible skills p because you have decided and your parents have decided that you want to be a 25 different type of thing, which 132 1 is a little bit more of a generalist, rather than a specialist at S and so there's going to be literally thousands 5 of these, so here you can make someone all of a sudden be able to recognize 100 objects, know exactly how many there are, and it's latent in all of you it's 10 in your brain, but you have grown up in such a way that you've made yourself stupid in that way, OK? Fabulous. So for most jobs you don't need to know 15 how many things there are, so we're fine. And God good receptionists, don't remember that NUMEROSITY. But being an astro physicist. It's really 20 going to happen. So the argument in this field now is before we start fixing people and saying you have a disability and you don't, we need to think 25 what is the diversity here in 133 1 the kinds of skills people have, and are we underrecognizing things that are really valuable and not finding the kinds of 5 jobs and the kinds of things and doing teaching the way you do in ways that actually are responsive to differences which are very strong and are not just 10 worse. People are walking around that do poorly in your classes who are smarter than you by far in other ways. That's what's the striking finding. 15 And it's coming out everywhere. OK, so the argument is neurodiversity and if you look up neurotypicals, Google neurotypicals, you'll find 20 hysterical people talking about you as if you have major problems and don't you wish you could be as da da da da da as people on autism spectrum 25 disorder? 134 1 >> >>: lastly, oh, so sorry, to make that point, that from the days in which the nimas laws were written, where we saw 5 disability as broken and as resonant in children or adults, something they have like an illness, the movement of universal design, the movement 10 of science, is toward a recognition that there's great diversity amongst us. And the idea that you can separate out and say, those are disabled and 15 those are not, isn't going to hold water in the future. So we're not going to be able to define them by organic. We're not even going to be able to 20 define them by you have to have a disability, because someone's going to come up to you and say OK, tell me how many things I threw on the table and you're 25 going to go I don't know, 3434? 135 1 No, it's 97, you have a disability. That we're not going to be able to do this in the long run. There isn't going 5 to be any bright line between ability and dissality, which is a good thing. >> >>: OK, and the big change 10 that cast focused on more than any other, is that the whole reason nimas points back to 1933 was the library of Congress, the recognition that books were not 15 usable by some people who had disabilities, and until there were alternatives, though, you really didn't -- you had to continue to think of a person as 20 having a disability and needing to the fixed and all of that. And with new media, we in fact have a different alternative, which is to say, actually books 25 aren't very good as 136 1 instructional media for anybody, and we need to change the kinds of things we use for instruction, which is what we're 5 looking at. So this I'm going to blitz through. The power of the media is the flexibility, we can store information in them, keep it permanent, but display 10 it in many ways, blah blah blah, we can take the same information and make it in in different colors, different fonts, anything like that as you know, 15 but you can also make it immediately into something you can touch as opposed to look. You can make it into something you can listen to by turning on 20 text to speech, you can turn it into ASL and that's the genius of nimas, will you give us one good digital version, we can from that make countless 25 versions, versions that talk, 137 1 versions that are bigger, versions that you can touch, versions that can talk to you with their hands, et cetera. 5 Those things are all possible if we start with a digital source rather than starting with a printed source. And for cast we're very interested in not 10 only can you make it physically and sense oral accessible, but can we provide supports directly in the material so we can in fact reduce threat. We can 15 increase challenges by saying you know what there are more resources, there's more help in the document itself so you don't have to go into threat. We also 20 think they make the better environments for learning, which is a longer argument. And I want to give a flavor of what are environments that are very 25 supportive look like. What are 138 1 these new environments going to look like. And I've forgotten, skip you told me and I don't remember. Did you show any of 5 the reading environments. >> >>: I went through just the sonnet yesterday 10 >> >>: and did that live, though? >> >>: yes. So I want to skip the universal learning editions 15 whereby you can say I need it to read to me, I need help with the vocabulary, all of those things. We can easily do that and this is just to show the guidelines 20 that talk about them. We want to make sure everybody can see it. So we can and I don't mean see t sorry, we want to make sure everybody can perceive it 25 so we can easily make things 139 1 talk, we can easily make them bigger, we can make them a different color, all of those things. We also want to make 5 sure that the language and symbols doesn't prevent kids. Some kids daunt decode. Easy enough to have a computer decode. It may be that English 10 is not their first language. As some of you know we're doing a project with Google will now and there's automatic translation to 42 languages instantaneous so 15 you can just so give that to me in my language and there's all sorts of other things we can do here, including not using language and symbols at all and 20 to provide options to make sure that the language and symbols around getting in the way and lastly things that we can do for comprehension. And a lot of 25 people don't have the same 140 1 background knowledge, something I talked about earlier, that the reason they can't understand it isn't because they can't see it, 5 isn't because they don't understand the language and symbols, but they don't have the background knowledge, but we can build that in. It's very simple 10 to build that in nowadays and we can highlight critical features, so that you can have a start to get into things and so on. Lots of options. So the UDL 15 guidelines which I'll show you how to get, talk about how do you present information with enough options that everybody can get to it, that we're not 20 decreasing the oxygen? So just in the spirit of what's next, I want to show you not one of our own, but something that's come out fairly recently from 25 scholastic for -- let's see 141 1 where is it? Sorry, one second. I guess I shut down by mistake. Don't look at my codes. So I just want to give you a sense of 5 what is this world coming to in terms of how the things we use to learn in look different than a print world? And I'm sorry, I'm on a -- I seem to be on a 10 bit of a slow line here. So this is called expert space. And expert space is an information -- it's a -- the idea is how are we going to 15 teach students to be good at it, finding, evaluation and using information in this sort of media-rich world, all right? And you'll see here -- sorry. 20 >> >>: so here's topics. These they've done the most typical topics that people look at in middle school and high school, 25 you know, energy, da da da, 142 1 things pop up when you roll over them. There's many more. There's hundreds of them. Here's the interesting thing 5 about them. When I go to one, -- so let's see, I probably should be careful, but we'll go to one endangered species, and sorry things are slow. When it 10 opens up, this opens up into an environment where students can learn about endangered species. I've never seen it be quite this slow. I wonder if I'm really 15 on. It keeps making and losing my connection. So this doesn't for some reason, can someone get the tech person just to see why I dome don't seem to be -- do 20 you know what's happening in a way that I don't? >> >>: it's updating. 25 >> >>: oh, it's up now. OK, all 143 1 right, thank you. I probably won't try to do everything. >> >>: let me show you how this 5 works. Because it has a lot of. How do you begin? It begins with a gorgeous video, Hollywood style. I'm not going to play it because it feels like the line 10 is slow for some reason, but it orant you to endangered species, get out the vocabulary get out some ideas, so you're in a space where you feel like you have 15 some resourcesment remember what I was talking about earlier. So it says not starting by reading, let's give you some background information and then the next 20 thing it does is says OK, you want to read some more about it, and we come down here and say yeah, I want to read some more. 25 >> >>: I apologize for whatever 144 1 is making this -- look what's interesting about this. So here's an article. It's like an encyclopedias article. But 5 what's the difference? For one thing, you can say what LEXILE level do I want to read this at? So every article. Does everybody know what LEXILE is? 10 LEXILE is one of the ways of judging how difficult an article is to read and so these are different levels from a much lower to a much higher so it 15 comes in at sort of an average. Then all of these are available. I can turn the read along. So it will just start reading to me. I can look up words 20 anywhere I want to, take notes, all of those things. Automatically I is part of it. I just show you a couple more things. So I'm in a standard, 25 this is like going to an 145 1 encyclopedia, and but I'm never going to get left alone in terms of there being lots of support available to me. And let me go 5 back, so that's a an introductory article, so I've been able to see a video, now read an article, what's next? What's really cool about it is 10 that the next thing that happens is you see dive deeper. Now, what happens is then there's a gradual release from first a video, a very supported reading 15 environment, where every word and there's comprehension supports and things built into it t but then dive deeper, there's thousands of articles. 20 They've got GROLLIER's encyclopedia and put them all in here so there's plenty of information. So it's like saying let's say you come in 25 with a background knowledge and 146 1 you can dive deeper and ever yon one of them is LEXILEed. So I'm not going to get shut because of my reading ability or I can't 5 see it or whatever, but there's actually more than 90,000 articles that are all prepared in this way. 10 >> >>: now, that says there's remember three parts of the brain so I'm just going to show you just talk about this in those -- first, it says this 15 information should be available in multiple ways that you can get to it. Watch it, you can read it, you can read it with a lot of support, always ask for 20 vocabulary and decoding support et cetera. Oh, somewhere in there, it does do you want it in Spanish. I don't see it. So you can say I want it in my 25 first language, et cetera. All 147 1 built in. That's only the back part of your brain. It says everybody ought to at least know what this information is. We 5 can do that. The good part is how does it deal with the front part of the brain, helping kids be more strategic, acting on this information, finding good 10 stuff. So all of that is built up here, that there are tools and resources to help you learn how to use them. There's a great note-taking which allows 15 you to -- it's fabulous, because it allows you to take notes but essentially just dragging it but the great thing is that it makes sure that you cite, it drags the 20 notes over and it says where did you get this? So it reminds kids, this is something they've copied. And it will help them then make that you are 25 bibliography. But it's a 148 1 teaching instrument. It says OK, you've used these sources. Now it's time to construct your bibliography, and believe me, I 5 don't have time to show this but I'm sure -- I have graduate students who do not know how to do proper citation, so built into it are the common like MLA 10 and stuff citation links that help them learn what is the way to do a bibliography, all part of this built in and I'll just show you one example. If you 15 have trouble getting ideas, you can go in here and say, ah, we'll help you get some ideas. There's dictionaries and things like that and an outline builder 20 which is really cool. You can drag things around and it's like a concept map thing. All built in. And I just want to show you, I think what the kids find 25 most helpful is the strategy and 149 1 skill building stuff. Here's some of the things that are here. There's more coming, but here's the things that they 5 found that students again, middle school, high school, but I have to say, I find it in graduate will school, that how to set good goals for a lesson. 10 As you know, half the time the kids said well I want to learn about and it's the topic that is like impossible. Because it's too big or sometimes too narrow. 15 Usually too big. So how do you set even a good goal for setting a topic? How do you search in a web environment. And it really tutors them and gives them 20 practice at saying what's a good question you could ask of a Google search engine so you get good information instead of crappy information. How do you 25 evaluate sources to decide 150 1 what's good and bad? I'll show what it looks like when you do it. These two are the best ones, note taking, the kids are 5 terrible note takers, so there's this fabulous long do it with us, we're going to model how we take notes, you do it, we do it, you go back and forth. And then 10 it even gives you feedback, soak, it OK, it looks like you took too much. Outlining, citing sources. I just want to show you what it looks like when 15 you go into one of these. Citing sources my graduate students, I want them to all to take this. 20 >> >>: hey, glad you're back the I'm HEDRON and I'll here to help you get your projects together. Today we're going to go over citing sources. If you want to 25 pause or view a section again, 151 1 click the tab up here. >> >>: you have to cite your sources whenever you take notes. 5 Check out the note-taking skill builder to get a refresher on that key skill. Signing a source means to give credit to the author and publisher of the 10 information you're using. We acknowledge their ideas and the research and citations are our note cards. When we finish a project, research paper or 15 homework assignment, we've put all these citations together in a bibliography. I think doing research is kind of like building a team, a team of 20 experts who help me with the facts and ideas I need for my paper or project. The more experts I have, the stronger my presentation. When I cite a 25 source, that author joins my 152 1 team of experts in my bibliography. If I do a good job bringing my team together, we can't be beat, and my paper 5 will be a winner. Sometimes people don't give credit when they copy words, facts or ideas from a book or a website. That's called plagiarism. 10 Plagiarism is a serious offense with serious consequences for students and researchers. But hey, don't worry, I've got a game plan that will make it easy 15 for you to give ... >> >>: OK, so you can see so he talks for a while but then he gets you doing T he says, OK, 20 you try it. I'm going to give you an example. You try citing some sources and then we'll get your feedback and so on. But what they have is a gradually 25 released space which goes from 153 1 building background knowledge before you start it, trying out some things, and then some articles that are very heavily 5 and carefully scaffolded to a broader set of articles. I left out one more step sort of mid-level articles that are sort of medium in their expanse and 10 then out to 90,000 articles, with the capacity to help you learn how to search, learn how to cite, learn how to gather, learn how to take notes, all of 15 these things. So these kinds of environment, I think I just wanted to show you a recent example. Every word 20 >> >>: you've got a question. >> >>: you need to provide captioning. You've already included. English as a second 25 language. How we show -- then 154 1 the >> >>: I believe they are captioned, and I don't know how 5 to turn them on, but >> >>: that doesn't have it. I mean the video you showed earlier, but this one, I don't 10 see, happens you >> >>: all right, I'll check, because that would be problematic if it doesn't. 15 Because this, the yes, right, the video, with the captions on were the captions on the videos when they played automatically? I forget. 20 >> >>: no, I mean on this one. Into it didn't have captions on. >> >>: it didn't have captions 25 on. 155 1 >> >>: yeah, OK, well all the videos are captioned whether these tutorials are, I don't know -- I'd have to check. But 5 when those opening videos came on, they had the captions turned on, yes? >> >>: we watched a video. 10 >> >>: oh, I didn't turn it on. Oh, I'm sorry, I didn't turn it on the video. They are captioned and so you can turn 15 the captions on and off. But whether these are, I am sea not sure. Whether the tutorial and I apologize for not knowing that. Awe all of the video 20 introductions to all of the novels, they are all captioned. >> >>: and but thank you for mentioning it and I'll check. 25 We told them to caption 156 1 everything. In fact, I should say, one of the -- I'll get back to it. 5 >> >>: OK. So let me get back to my slides. >> >>: similarly, you would take and I think skip did a little 10 bit of this, look at expression, and say what are the things that might interfear with a person being able to express what they know? And we literally move 15 around the ring and say, well, what about physical action? The ability to actually act, so then there are options that allow a person who is physically 20 disabled like Matthew, the guy I showed, to be able to do that. Secondly, the ability to write and draw means bothing be able to move the instruments, to be 25 able to spell correctly, to be 157 1 able to organize and do all of those. Are there options available in the media, so in the program I just showed you, 5 actually an outlining tool is built in to provide some support for kids and they can type and so on in a variety of ways and lastly, importantly, executive 10 functions. The ability to set proper goals, to make a good plan, so in that program I just showed, one of the problems that kids really have is they have 15 trouble getting a good plan for what they're going to write about and search for and so on so it actually tutors them and helps them and guides they will 20 them to how do I build a plan. >> >>: and similarly we can look at -- I want to skip these for a moment. I left off the affect. 25 The program I just showed how 158 1 does it go after affect? A it starts with the fun stuff to start with, but it says you have a lot of options to choose which 5 would be supportive, what would help you, and it allows kids, one of the most important things is to allow kids to set thousand those helps and I'm a little bit 10 behind and I apologize so I wanted to just go to a few things that get me to my course. In this environment, though, I want to say that print looks 15 much more disabled when we can't do these things. Or as you pointed out, if the videos are not captioned, then they're disabled. They place people in 20 a position where they can't in fact get the information out of them. And print has that problem in spades. A lot of people can't use print of 25 course, and the disabilities, 159 1 the change in the viewpoint that we have from instead of seeing kids as having disabilities, to seeing the media as having 5 disabilities is an important change. Disabilities in who they can teach, we can't reach all of our kids when we have disabled media. There's not 10 enough oxygen in the air. They're disabled in what we can teach. There are topics which are not well done in present. Mathematics is not well taught 15 in printed textbooks. Most science and so on. So they have disabilities in the kinds of things they can teach. They can't teach kids, in fact, to be 20 skillful, to be able to do things. They're really just information retrievable devices and lastly they don't prepare students for their future. This 25 is a picture of my daughter 160 1 who's just finishing medical school, and most of what she does now is not -- they don't even assign any textbooks. 5 Largely she's on computers the whole time. She is -- everybody gets a laptop. They do all their work that day, they do simulations, they do surgeries 10 on computer and so on. And that by preparing kids only with paper we're not preparing them for the future in which they're going to live. 15 >> >>: so in this modern world, it's not who has print disabilities, but the key thing is what has print disabilities. 20 So in terms of this law back from the beginning, the question is who has a print disability and I would want to reframe it in saying our schools have print 25 disabilities. They're not able 161 1 to do the things they need to do. They're not able to reach all the students, they're not able to teach all their subjects 5 and they're not preparing their kids for the future. That's too much disability. I want, just to finish up, I want to talk about my own course a little 10 bit. >> >>: so this will look ugly because the course just finished and there's a lot of emails at 15 the end. But I want to show you some things from my teaching and talk about some things that worked and didn't work. This is a main blog and I know that I 20 just heard you're doing a blog, and you can see a few things just happened and they look boring and they're notes and stuff to the students. But 25 every student had a blog in the 162 1 course. So here's all my students. And this provided an alternate way of interacting with the course. Which is to 5 say that there were things that happened in lecture where we all did them together. On their blogs, people took a much more individual attack on the course. 10 That is, they did the things that made the course meaningful to them. And in the media that were valuable and interesting and usable by them. So they 15 looked very different, one from another. I'll just show a couple of the blogs to give you a sense of blogs from I ended up seeing these students, and this 20 one is someone who's quite FACILE with modern technology so his is laden with gorgeous videos, commentaries, he would half of my lectures and make 25 them better by saying you know, 163 1 if David had had this, it would have been a lot better and it's just full of resources for my teaching next year. I mean I 5 don't know how many probably 35 or 50 posts and it's full of things that I could use. He even gives me highlighting critical features, he highlights 10 things for me, make sure I'll see them and makes tutorials and stuff. So that that's a, it goes on and on, it's just amazing. And in this realm, I 15 put much less what should I say top-down influence on what should be on the blog. A basically asked them to respond and think about things, the 20 topics in the course, respond to each other's blogs and so on? OK, and they're really fabulous, I have to say. I mean I want to go back and actually look at 25 them all, and I would say 10 164 1 percent of them will probably keep them as their permanent blogs. That is, that they're already going on each other's 5 blogs and talking about p stuff and so on. I want to go to my slides to show you another one. Two other tools that I used a lot was that I used book 10 builder. How many people have seen book builder? So quite a few. So book builder, very simple tool. Originally made for first grade teachers to use 15 in making books that had multiple supports built in for students who have intellectual disabilities but it's taken off as just a think that you can 20 make cool things on the web with that are highly supported. Among other things, it allows you to make a -- put a little mentor in who talks. All you 25 have to do is type in what you 165 1 want him to say and he says it. Multiple after avatars, you can make your own face be the avatar and so on. You can I am bed 5 questions and queries and so on in these very simple books cht and I just keep this one up because I wanted to -- well, this so you that sometimes 10 people have done things that are very college-looking. Again it's made for first graders. So here's a book that this person made on quadratic functions. 15 It's got you know, your joke to begin. And here's, you just move pages like this, you see there's a little -- this is a little read me this aloud, give 20 me some definition, all those kinds of things are up here. But this one is ugly, that is, it's not beautifully designed, but it's just this rich thing 25 about quadratic equations and I 166 1 just -- I didn't actually grade this one, but I wanted to show you what he's been able to embed a fabulous movie videos, things 5 you couldn't possibly do in print. I want to get to one that I might understand it. >> >>: I guess it was that one. 10 >> >>: and he did this so I'm not sure, the student did this so I'm not sure this is captioned, either. Too slow. 15 I'm going to let that go in the background. I don't know, from my hotel, all this worked very fast. Let me see if that will load while I'm doing something 20 else. Everything is slow. >> >>: you might have to close some of the programs. 25 >> >>: oh, good idea. I might 167 1 have too much going here. >> >>: I have a lot going. Are you going to help me? That 5 would be great. I'd love it. >> >>: oh, go to the same place on another sheet? Uh-oh, then I'd have to be able to give you 10 the URL. >> : OK. He says that other machine has a faster connection. So that would be fabulous. Mine 15 seems to be jumping off. Oh, it's hopelessly difficult. >> >>: all right, I apologize. I don't know why. See this 20 worries me, it's saying that every little >> >>: just turn off your wireless connection, you won't 25 get that message any more. But 168 1 then actually leave the Internet and turn that back on. >> >>: I think I'm going to let 5 him play with it and let me say some things. Two things I want to show are book builder which I can show later which is this thing that makes it easy to make 10 books that are rich in media and that have built-in supports so that you can make them. They're not going to be quite as richly textured as the thing that 15 psychological asic scholastic can do with a lot of money but nonetheless there's 2,000 books made by teachers that are shared in a public library and there 20 are 18,000 books that teachers have made that are available just really to their classes. So you can make digital books that talk and breathe and all 25 that that you can share with 169 1 just your classroom or you can share with the rest of the world and they've become sort of a hot thing. So just to say what's 5 next with that, we're actually working with Google to bring out an authoring system could he shah so that people can make industrial strength really rich 10 books with lots of features in them freely available to everybody. So that project is not quite finished but at some point Google will hopefully make 15 it very clear to you and that will be something that it makes it easy for you to both make things and share them with your friends and colleagues. 20 >> >>: did you switch machines? >> >>: I didn't have the sign-on codes. 25 170 1 >> >>: OK, so here's book builder, you learn about UDL in it, you can see some model books, you can create your own 5 books, you can share books with just our own class, or you can look for books in the public library. Which are ones that have been shared by other 10 teachers, and that's where the quadratic function, whatever that means. Everything seems a little slow. Here's all the books. Sorry, here's how you 15 get to all the books. And I'll -- let me just find one that I don't have to think about. Oh, look at the titles. A brief history of assistive 20 technology in education. There's sort of random assortment here, but I can go put in a title, put in an author, put in an illustrator, 25 my school, I can ask for grade 171 1 levels, I can say I want content areas, so we can ask for science, and go tand get just books on science, books for 6th 5 grade science or whatever, so all of them have been made by teachers, educators of some kind, and put in the -- put here where you can use them and add 10 to them yourself. And I'll sorry I'll go back and just open up a piece of one book and I apologize for blowing my time here. But this is something you 15 can easily pull up. Perhaps I'll do this in the afternoon session to get a sense for what's here. And -- well, look at this, who knows what this is 20 about. I mean I know what it's about, but I don't know if it's any good. I can just say I want to read that book and you'll see there's a tool bar that comes 25 up, there's support for 172 1 vocabulary and things like that and of course this could be a terrible book, I haven't seen this one. A brief history. 5 There's a recording of voice here. A dog. And no mentors yet. All right, so so far not a very exciting book. So this is someone I think is experimenting 10 doing their first book. So typically there would be mentors and things that would support you down here, and this one is just beginning. And I realize 15 I'm at the end of my time. The what I'd like to do this afternoon is show some more tools and things that people can use. So there are commercially 20 available things that are increasingly coming out that are examples of UDL and you can increasingly use tools. Google will be coming out with some 25 where you can make your own 173 1 stuff, and I want to say one thing that didn't work in my class. Which when people have their own blogs, I encourage 5 people to look and comment on each other's blogs, and what was interesting to me is that the same cliquishness aRose that happens in other social 10 environments. I see people nodding so you've done this, but some people's blogs got almost no comments at all, and they were just as hurt as people who 15 nobody talked to after class. And you know, I could say everything I wanted as a professor up front, please comment on lots of people's, but 20 what I saw was that they aggregated into groups, and that people commented back and forth on each other's blogs and not on, some blogs just didn't have 25 it. Now, part of that is the 174 1 user's social function, some people knew how to make fabulous blogs that everybody commented on but some of it had to do with 5 the social structure that had nothing to do with the blogs and some kids just weren't getting the attention. The second thing that didn't work was I did a 10 module which worked in terms of, it worked, but it brought up a problem I never thought of. Which is that people read and they highlighted and they did 15 things within it which was much better than a book in some ways. It could talk, it could do all those things. But it allowed me to see everything a student did 20 in the book. When they highlighted it, when they asked for help, when they made a note to commentary, all of those things, which sounded great. 25 But as I was doing it, I 175 1 realized, I felt vaguely -- not vaguely, I felt intrusive, like I knew too much because I could literally see when did they 5 study, how long did they read this book? Did they get help? Did they turn on the speech? Did they -- I could know everything. And I had to kind 10 of call a class and say, I'll not going to look at that any more. I could literally see what they high highlighted and make comments on their 15 highlighting and I just didn't know whether that was OK, because is it OK to be a stownt and not do the reading? Because you have other ways of doing 20 well in the course? And I would know that they never did the reading. Or I would know that they don't know how to highlight. And I didn't know. 25 So it was really kind of 176 1 something new and I knew literally that they studied, I couldn't tell if they studied at midnight only because the 5 environment kept track of every key stroke and so I had this ethical problem of I don't know if I should know every key stroke that a student makes. So 10 just an interesting thing, perhaps you'll talk about it at the -- for those of you who come to the afternoon session when we talk about it, it's very 15 provocative at the college level. What does it mean if you know everything? Anyway, thank you very much. Facile. Facile. 20 >> >>: thank you so much David. It's a wonderful and exciting morning and I'm glad that you're going to be able to stay with us a little bit longer after lunch. 25 Lunch will be out in the hallway 177 1 and you can come back if had here if if you'd like or find another location to have your lunch. We invite you to visit 5 the exhibiter hall over the lunch hour, and then at 1:00, please find your way to your breakout session. You should have an envelope, if you're new 10 today, an envelope which has the breakout session name and number that you registered for, and if you have any question about the location of that, there are 15 schedules that indicate the room numbers or the room names, rather, for the breakout session, and there are signs in the hallway that should be 20 fairly easy to find. Feel free to ask anyone with a yellow tag and they'll help you to find your way. 25 >> >>: can I say one more thing 178 1 is it >> >>: I just realized I wanted to say something. Thank you for 5 pointing out that that part wasn't captioned. I wanted to say one thing about captions, which is really the future next step. Because it shows where 10 universal design works. As you probably know, Google will videos how will automatically caption themselves. Have you seen that so you can just say 15 caption my video, and of course that's a benefit for people who are deaf. But what it also allows you to do is to search for videos by the words because 20 now they have words attached to them. And I just wanted to point out it's a good example of the advantages of doing UDL provide more ramps for a lot 25 more people, so in fact, now 179 1 Google has a better way and YouTube to find the right videos, because in fact more and more of them are being 5 captioned. Now it's very automatic. You should ask people to caption the videos, because it's actually fairly simple to do so. Anyway, 10 thanks. >> >>: one of your breakout sessions is on that topic, that captions are not just for deaf, 15 so you might want to check that out. Those of you who were looking for a clue about the prize that will be given this evening, very valuable prize, 20 there is a clue, I believe, on the blog, so you can check out the clue on the blog, but if you'd liked this morning's session, you will love this 25 prize. Enjoy your lunch ...: 180 1 Test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test 5 test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test 10 test test test test test test test test test test test test test test test 15 25